Different names for “natural gas” influence public perception of it

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101671Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Americans have strong positive feelings for the term “natural gas”.

  • Americans have negative views of the alternative terms “methane” and “methane gas”.

  • They associate natural gas with “clean” and methane gas with “pollution”.

  • The alternative terms “fossil gas” and “fracked gas” are politically polarized.

Abstract

In many countries, natural gas is perceived more favorably than other fossil fuels. Here, we experimentally test (N = 2931) how perceptions of natural gas vary depending on what it is called. We find that Americans have stronger positive feelings for the term “natural gas” than “natural methane gas” (d = 0.59), “fossil gas” (d = 0.80), “fracked gas” (d = 0.81), “methane” (d = 0.94), and “methane gas” (d = 0.96). Democrats and Republicans both reported more positive views of “natural gas” than “natural methane gas” or “methane [gas].” But the patterns for the two political parties differed for perceptions of “fossil gas” and “fracked gas,” which were both viewed relatively positively by Republicans but negatively by Democrats. Analyses of open-ended word associations found that many participants associated methane with words like “pollution” and “global warming,” whereas they associated natural gas with words like “clean.” The results suggest that the terms used for this fossil fuel have very different meanings among the public, which may affect people's risk perceptions, consumer choices, and support for related policies.

Introduction

Natural gas is primarily (70–90%) composed of methane, a greenhouse gas. Methane has approximately 25 times more global warming potential than carbon dioxide for a time horizon of 100 years, meaning that 1 ton of methane absorbs approximately 25 times more energy (i.e., traps more heat or causes more radiative forcing) than 1 ton of carbon dioxide (Environmental Protection Agency, 2020; Forster et al., 2007). In 2015, the amount of methane that leaked from oil and natural gas production in the United States (U.S.) was estimated to be equivalent to about 2.3% of gross production (i.e., 7.6 million tons leaked vs. 33 million tons produced; Alvarez et al., 2018). Over a 20-year time horizon, the global warming impact of annual fugitive methane emissions from the oil and gas supply chain in the U.S. is roughly equivalent to the carbon dioxide released from all U.S. coal-fired power plants operating in 2015 (Alvarez et al., 2018). In other words, methane leaks “substantially erode the potential climate benefits of natural gas use” (Alvarez et al., 2018, p. 3).

Yet, whereas coal is viewed by many Americans as a harmful energy source, perceptions of natural gas are much more positive. A majority (76%) of the American public have favorable attitudes toward natural gas (Climate Nexus et al., 2020), and this is consistent across many countries around the world (Kennedy et al., 2020). The American public's attitudes toward natural gas are closer to their attitudes toward wind and solar (82% and 87% in favor; Climate Nexus et al., 2020) than to coal (39% in favor), even though electricity production from natural gas is responsible for 10–44 times more greenhouse gas emissions than electricity produced from wind and solar (Schlömer et al., 2014, Table A.III.2). On average, 62% of Americans (57% of Republicans and 76% of Democrats) think coal is harmful to human health, compared to only 29% (22% of Republicans and 35% of Democrats) who think natural gas is (Leiserowitz et al., 2018). In contrast, when asked about landfill methane gas as an energy source, 42% of Americans (39% of Republicans and 50% of Democrats) think it is harmful to human health, suggesting that the term “landfill methane gas” is perceived more negatively than “natural gas” (although this may be driven in part by the word “landfill”). This finding of diverging perceptions of similar energy sources (i.e., natural gas and landfill methane gas) raises the question of whether people's favorable perceptions of natural gas change depending on whether it is called “natural gas,” “methane gas,” or some other alternative label.

Labels function as shorthand symbols that help convey meaning, and they can work similarly to emphasis framing when they highlight different underlying aspects of an issue at the expense of other aspects (Cacciatore et al., 2016; Clarke et al., 2015; Entman, 1993). Names or labels can influence public opinion about an issue by making certain attributes of a product, object, event, or phenomenon more salient, thus activating different sets of affect and emotions, mental models, and risk perceptions (Clarke et al., 2015, Leiserowitz & Smith, 2017; Schuldt, 2016). When names or labels activate different perceptions of risk, they can profoundly shape consumer preferences, public policy support, and activism (Slovic, 1999). For example, the American public is more supportive of hydraulic fracturing when it is called “shale oil or gas development” than when it is called “fracking” (Clarke et al., 2015). Similarly, many studies demonstrate that food products labeled with the word “natural” are perceived to be better for the environment, healthier, tastier, and more moral (Asioli et al., 2017; Chambers et al., 2018; Sörqvist et al., 2015). Further, using a natural analogy to describe geoengineering technology (“like an artificial tree”) increases support for this technology (Corner & Pidgeon, 2015).

In a prior study, a small group of Americans (N = 94) were asked about the images they associate with “natural gas,” and whether those images were positive or negative. “Natural gas” elicited mostly neutral images (e.g., fire, pipe, gas appliances; Truelove, 2012). However, the sample was small, and the affective measure used may have obscured some important nuances, such as whether Americans hold ambivalent attitudes toward either natural gas or some of the images they associated with natural gas. This has important implications because ambivalent attitudes are less stable over time and are more susceptible to persuasion (Conner & Armitage, 2008). Ambivalent attitudes are also weaker predictors of behavioural intentions compared to non-ambivalent attitudes (Costarelli & Colloca, 2004). Further, this prior study did not address other important questions, such as how people respond to alternative terms for natural gas (e.g., methane gas), and how much people's perceptions are driven by these word choices (such as the use of the word “natural”). The current study addresses these questions in a large, randomized experiment (N = 2931).

This study assesses positive and negative affect associated with six different names for natural gas. The name “natural gas” refers to a fossil fuel gas mixture that contains 70–90% methane. It was named as such to differentiate it from artificial or manufactured gas, which was used throughout the nineteenth century (Castaneda, 2001; Waples, 2012). More recently, the natural gas industry has branded it as “cleaner energy,” “the cleanest fossil fuel,” and has promoted cooking with gas as being “synonymous with speed and efficiency” (American Gas Association, 2021a, 2021b; Enbridge, 2021). Although “methane gas” is not frequently used in public discourse to refer to natural gas, it is included in this study as an alternative term that emphasizes the primary compound in natural gas. Although not a term widely used in current discourse, “natural methane gas” is included to isolate the labeling effect associated with the word “natural.” Similarly, “methane” is included to isolate any potential effects related to the word “gas.” Climate advocates have started using the terms “fracked gas” (e.g., Sierra Club, 2018; #frackedgas on Twitter) and “fossil gas” (Global Witness, 2021) as alternative names for natural gas. The term “fossil gas” emphasizes that natural gas, like coal and oil, is a fossil fuel. The term “fracked gas” emphasizes a method of natural gas extraction using hydraulic fracturing or fracking, and aims to activate mental associations related to fracking, which tend to be more negative than positive (Clarke et al., 2013, 2015). Because these are currently contested terms, it is important to subject them to empirical tests.

Little is known about how people feel about the different terms that can be used to describe natural gas or whether different terms elicit different reactions among Republicans and Democrats. We tested three hypotheses. First, we predicted that “natural gas” would be viewed more positively than “methane” or “methane gas.” Second, we predicted that natural methane gas” would be viewed more positively than “methane” or “methane gas.” Third, we predicted that Republicans would have more positive attitudes overall compared to Democrats, similar to the aforementioned political party differences in perceived harm to health (Leiserowitz et al., 2018). The terms “fossil gas” and “fracked gas” were also included because these terms are increasingly being used by climate advocates, but we made no specific predictions about the feelings and images associated with these two terms.

Another objective of this study was to better understand what comes to mind when people think of natural gas and other names for it. Affective image analysis and word association techniques provide unique insights because they help reveal the mental representations held by a population in a way that is “unfiltered, relatively context-free, and spontaneous” (Leiserowitz & Smith, 2017). We conducted exploratory analyses to answer the following questions: What associations (words or phrases) come to mind most often in each condition? Do these associations generate positive or negative affect? Finally, we examined how people perceive the relationship between natural gas and methane to determine whether the American public understands that natural gas is comprised primarily of methane.

Section snippets

Power and sequential analyses

Sequential analyses allows researchers to conduct high-powered experiments efficiently (i.e., can reduce the required sample size by 30% or more; Lakens, 2014), while controlling for error rates. Based on a smallest effect size of interest (SESOI) set at d = 0.15, a power analysis indicated that a total of 699 participants per condition was needed (alpha = .05, power = .80, two-tailed). We were willing to collect up to 699 participants per condition but chose to stop earlier if a significant

Variations in affect across conditions

To investigate variations in affective responses to different terms for natural gas, we asked participants to rate their positive and negative feelings regarding “natural gas,” “natural methane gas,” “methane gas,” “methane,” “fossil gas,” and “fracked gas.” Participants (N = 2931) were randomly assigned to one of the six conditions. Scores for the positive and negative affect items were averaged, and the average affect was entered as the dependent variable in our analyses.

As illustrated in

Discussion

Natural gas is the dominant source of primary energy used in the industrial, residential, and commercial sectors in the U.S. (U.S. Energy Information Administration, 2019). Even though natural gas is primarily methane, this study indicates that Americans have a much more positive view of “natural gas” than “methane” or “methane gas.” This is likely due in part to the use of the key word natural in “natural gas.” Americans' views of “fossil gas” and “fracked gas” are politically polarized; these

Contributions

All authors (K.L., M.H.G., A.G., S.A.R., A.L.) conceived and designed the study. K.L., M.H.G., and A. G., collected the data. K.L. analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. K.L. interpreted the results together with M.H.G. All authors revised the manuscript.

Declaration of competing interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Acknowledgment

The research was funded by the High Tide Foundation, the Energy Foundation, the MacArthur Foundation, and the 11th Hour Project. We thank our independent coder Francis Commerçon.

References (42)

  • M.A. Cacciatore et al.

    The end of framing as we know it … and the future of media effects

    Mass Communication & Society

    (2016)
  • C. Castaneda

    Manufactured and natural gas industry

    (2001)
  • V.E. Chambers et al.

    What is “natural”? Consumer responses to selected ingredients

    Foods

    (2018)
  • C. Clarke et al.

    Fracking in the American mind: Americans' views on hydraulic fracturing in september, 2012

    (2013)
  • Climate NeCxus

    Yale program on climate change communication, & George Mason University center for climate change communication

  • J.G. CNN

    Global methane emissions are at a record high, and burping cows are driving the rise

  • M. Conner et al.

    Attitude ambivalence

  • A. Coppock

    Generalizing from survey experiments conducted on mechanical Turk: A replication approach

    Political Science Research and Methods

    (2019)
  • A. Corner et al.

    Like artificial trees? The effect of framing by natural analogy on public perceptions of geoengineering

    Climatic Change

    (2015)
  • Enbridge

    Is natural gas environmentally-friendly?

  • R.M. Entman

    Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm

    Journal of Communication

    (1993)
  • Cited by (10)

    • Exposure to nature leads to a stronger natural-is-better bias in Chinese people

      2022, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      We further build on work regarding the association between connectedness to nature and naturalness bias (Li & Cao, 2020). Natural-is-better bias (or natural preference, see Scott & Rozin, 2020 for a more balanced view), referring to an inherent preference for items labeled as natural, pervades in a rich variety of domains from food to source of achievement to injury sources (Kellert & Wilson, 1993; Lacroix, Goldberg, Gustafson, Rosenthal, & Leiserowitz, 2021; Meier, Dillard, & Lappas, 2019; Rozin et al., 2004). This bias causes individuals to believe that products labeled as natural are better and safer than products without that label (Meier & Lappas, 2016); to favor naturally talented performers over less-talented strivers even when their professional performance is equal (Tsay & Banaji, 2011); and to perceive natural causes of injury to be less dangerous and scary in comparison to artificial causes (Rudski, Osei, Jacobson, & Lynch, 2011).

    • The political history of fossil fuels: Coal, oil, and natural gas in global perspective

      2023, Handbook on the Geopolitics of the Energy Transition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text